Unformatted text preview:

AST 309L Genomic perspective on convergence of intelligence (based on 11/20 lecture) This is a summary of the lecture on genomic aspects of “intelligence,” material that is not in the textbook. We began with a clarification about the similarity of the pygmy chimp and human genome—98% similarity still leaves a huge number of base pairs different, enough for thousands of genes. It is a myth that there intelligence or any human distinction must be expressed in a tiny part of the genome based on this similarity. Instead we emphasized that the similarity (or the 64% similarity with the mouse genome) strongly supports the idea that we had a common ancestor. On the other hand, I did present lots of evidence that the basis for human-like intelligence does reside on an even smaller part of the genome than anyone previously thought. That is the main subject here. We expected to find the “complexity” of “higher” animals and the “intelligence” of humans and perhaps a few other animals, to be reflected as a longer genome, with more DNA base pairs, or more genes to code for complex functions. However our conclusions point us in another direction:  Complexity of humans and other apparently “complex” organisms is not reflected in the number of base pairs in the genome, the “size” or “length” of the genome, nor is it reflected in the number of coding genes, which are the units of production of function (by coding for proteins). An implication we must take seriously for later is that we may be naïve to think that a big brain suggests high intelligence, just as we thought we thought high complexity would be reflected in a large genome.  The other conclusion is a puzzle: If not genome size, then where are the “instructions” for the complexity stored? What part of the genome codes for bipedal posture, big brains, opposable thumbs, complex neural circuitry, … intelligence? One possibility is that most of the complex “action” in the genome is taken by the “mobilome” of transposable elements and other dynamical processes, not in coding genes.  We also gave several examples that show that a complex trait can be carried by a small number of mutations, or even a single mutation, in a single gene. The examples included a number of diseases, but also camera eyes (PAX-6 gene), the development of plants that all animals require (through the symbiosis of plants with bacteria to utilize the nitrogen cycle), and size of cerebral cortex (ASPM gene) and skull (MYH16 gene mutation, disabling large and powerful jaw muscles). Actually this is a bit of a simplification—there are over a hundred genes that have been tentatively implicated as related to cognition, but ASPM and MYH16 are the most important by far. This seems to imply that factors leading to complexity, and intelligence, are contingent, since they depend on single mutations, and mutations are chance events. From one perspective, this is true, because out of the astronomical number of mutations that have occurred during the history of life, we have no way to say whether any particular mutation would occur again. (We could go further with this but it would require too much math.) However it is not true that these single mutations mean there has been no convergence. There are theoretical (and experimental) reasons for thinking that a rare but advantageous mutation will spread throughout a population, and it is a fundamental mistake to think that the same mutation had to occur independently in several creatures (e.g. the eight species with camera eyes), which is part of what suggested contingency to us. Instead, all that is required is that the mutation occurred in a distant (in time) enough common ancestor. Now recall the statement at the beginning, that this is what is implied by the genomic similarities of humans, chimps, mice, and other creatures. This was the case for the eyeball example, and for the plant-bacteria symbiosis example.What we have not answered (yet) is whether the mutation(s) for big brain, etc. leading to our kind of intelligence arose far back in our lineage (say a common ancestor like prosimians). If so, why don’t all the members of this lineage share these features that lead to intelligence? If instead, the mutation occurred recently, say five million years ago (e.g. giving rise to bipedality in only one line of creatures so far), why did it take so long? In this case humans could be the only creatures with intelligence on Earth at present. (Discounting crows and other cognitively-able organisms.) But now we can see that this does not suggest that extraterrestrial intelligence is improbable—it only shows that we neglected to even consider that humans are probably not the end of the line for the development of life on Earth. In particular, if our “lineage” continues, with branchings to other distinctive (and unimaginable) creatures in the future, they will indeed inherit those genes with the particular mutations that gave us all those traits we have discussed, and more. The “and more” is the most fascinating part, because we have no way to even guess what these future evolutionary changes may be, only that they will probably build on what exists here and now. Whatever they are, we only have to imagine a time in the distant future, after many more “branchings” have occurred in this lineage, to see that at that time “our kind of intelligence” will appear common, unless some other mutation or genome process eliminates it!  Remember the loss of the powerful jaws due to the MYH16 gene mutation! We concluded long ago, from the Drake equation, that if extraterrestrial civilizations are numerous enough so that the nearest average one is within a few hundred light years, their average time spent already as an “advanced civilization” must be very long, much longer than we have been a technological civilization. Our lowest estimates were about 100 thousand years. Putting these two results together, this shows how intelligence “like ours” could be convergent, even if humans are the only animals that possess it on Earth, and even if it depends on a few choice mutations, as long as we allow for the possibility that there is a lot of evolution yet to occur. Isn’t it peculiar that this “future evolution” is so rarely discussed? [See a book on the subject by Peter Ward if interested.] The major uncertainties in


View Full Document

UT AST 309L - LECTURE NOTES

Documents in this Course
Travel

Travel

31 pages

Life

Life

23 pages

Travel

Travel

31 pages

Life

Life

46 pages

Load more
Download LECTURE NOTES
Our administrator received your request to download this document. We will send you the file to your email shortly.
Loading Unlocking...
Login

Join to view LECTURE NOTES and access 3M+ class-specific study document.

or
We will never post anything without your permission.
Don't have an account?
Sign Up

Join to view LECTURE NOTES 2 2 and access 3M+ class-specific study document.

or

By creating an account you agree to our Privacy Policy and Terms Of Use

Already a member?