DOC PREVIEW
UA PSY 325 - Language in Context

This preview shows page 1-2-21-22 out of 22 pages.

Save
View full document
View full document
Premium Document
Do you want full access? Go Premium and unlock all 22 pages.
Access to all documents
Download any document
Ad free experience
View full document
Premium Document
Do you want full access? Go Premium and unlock all 22 pages.
Access to all documents
Download any document
Ad free experience
View full document
Premium Document
Do you want full access? Go Premium and unlock all 22 pages.
Access to all documents
Download any document
Ad free experience
View full document
Premium Document
Do you want full access? Go Premium and unlock all 22 pages.
Access to all documents
Download any document
Ad free experience
Premium Document
Do you want full access? Go Premium and unlock all 22 pages.
Access to all documents
Download any document
Ad free experience

Unformatted text preview:

Language in ContextOutline1. Reading: Bottom-Up and Top-Down ProcessesSlide 4Slide 52. Language and ThoughtSlide 7Slide 8Slide 9Slide 10Slide 11Slide 12Slide 133. Language in a Social ContextSlide 15Slide 16Slide 17Slide 18Slide 19Slide 203. Neuropsychology of LanguageSlide 22Language in ContextChapter 10Outline1. Reading: Bottom-Up and Top-Down Processes1. Perceptual Issues in Reading2. Lexical Processes in Reading2. Language and Thought1. Differences between Languages2. Bilingualism and Dialects3. Slips of the Tongue3. Language in a Social Context1. Speech Acts2. Conversational Postulates3. Gender and Language4. Neuropsychology of Language1. Reading: Bottom-Up and Top-Down Processes1. Perceptual Issues in Reading•Perception of orthographic aspects of words–Recognizing a particular letter when presented in visual form•Translating letters into phonological code–Translating the letter into the corresponding sound1. Reading: Bottom-Up and Top-Down Processes2. Lexical Processes in Reading–Put letters together and identify words•When reading our eyes move in saccades–Rapid sequential movements as they fixate on successive clumps of text–Saccadic movements leap an average of about 7 to 9 characters between successive fixations1. Reading: Bottom-Up and Top-Down Processes2. Lexical Processes in Reading•Lexical access –the identification of a word that allows us to gain access to the meaning of the word from memory•Word-superiority effect–Letters are read more easily when they are embedded in words than when they are presented either in isolation or with letters that do not form words•Sentence-superiority effect (Sentence context effect)–When a word (e.g. “window”) is standing by itself it is more difficult to recognize than when it is preceded by a sentence context such as “There were several repair jobs to be done. The first was to fix the ________.2. Language and Thought1. Differences between Languages•Differences in lexicon–People in Burma distinguish among many different kinds of rice–Nomadic Arabs have more than 20 different words for camels•Differences in syntax–Order of subject, verb, and object in a typical declarative sentence (e.g. Japanese vs. English)–Range of grammatical inflections and other markings (e.g. Russian vs. English)2. Language and Thought1. Differences between Languages•Linguistic Relativity: The Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis•The assertion that the speakers of different languages have differing cognitive systems and that these different cognitive systems influence the ways in which people speaking the various languages think about the world•Language shapes thought•Major proponents: Edward Sapir and Benjamin Lee Whorf2. Language and Thought1. Differences between Languages•Linguistic Relativity: The Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis–Current view – milder form •Language may not determine thought, but language certainly may influence thought•Language facilitates mental representation and manipulation•Language affects how we encode, store, and retrieve information in memory2. Language and Thought1. Differences between Languages•Linguistic universals–Characteristic patterns across languages of various cultures–Example•Languages name colors quite differently, but the languages do not divide the color spectrum arbitrarily•All languages seem to take their basic color terms from a set of just 11 color names organized into a hierarchy of five levels: (1) black, white; (2) red; (3) yellow, green, blue; (4) brown; and (5) purple, pink, orange, gray•If a language names only two colors they will be black and white, if it names three colors, they will be black, white and red2. Language and Thought2. Bilingualism and Dialects•Bilingualism–Bilinguals•People who can speak two languages•Simultaneous bilingualism occurs when a child learns two languages from birth•Sequential bilingualism occurs when an individual first learns one language and then another–Monolinguals•People who can speak only one language–In general it seems that bilingualism results in increased thinking ability2. Language and Thought2. Bilingualism and Dialects•Bilingualism –Single-system hypothesis•The two languages are represented in just one system–Dual-system hypothesis•The two languages are represented somehow in separate systems in the mind•When recovery of language after trauma is studied, sometimes the first language recovers first, sometimes the second language recovers first2. Language and Thought2. Bilingualism and Dialects•Language Mixtures and Change–Pidgin•When people of two different language groups are in prolonged contact with one another, the language users of the two groups begin to share some vocabulary that is superimposed onto each group’s syntax–Creole•When pidgin develops into a distinct linguistic form (new language) with its own grammar, it becomes a Creole–Dialect•A regional variety of a language distinguished by features such as vocabulary, syntax, and pronunciation2. Language and Thought3. Slips of the Tongue•Slips of the Tongue–Linguistic errors in what we say, which may occur at any level of linguistic analysis: phonemes, morphemes, or larger units of language•E.g. spoonerisms at the phonological level–“you have hissed all my mystery lectures”•E.g. Substitution at the semantic level –“after it is too late,” when you meant “before it is too late”3. Language in a Social Context-Pragmatics -the study of how people use language, including sociolinguistics and other aspects of the social context of language1. Speech Acts•Direct speech acts–The speech act is expressed directly (e.g. “Shut the door!”)•Indirect speech acts–We accomplish our goals in speaking in an oblique fashion (e.g. “Could you please shut the door?”)3. Language in a Social Context1. Speech Acts•Taxonomy of direct speech acts (Searle, 1975)–Representative•A speech act by which a person conveys a belief that a given proposition is true •E.g. “My students are smart.”–Directive•An attempt by a speaker to get a listener to do something, such as supplying the answer to question •E.g. “Shut the door!”3. Language in a Social Context1. Speech Acts•Taxonomy of direct speech acts (Searle, 1975)–Commissive•A commitment by the speaker to engage in some future course of action•E.g. “I’ll be there.”–Expressive•A statement regarding the speaker’s


View Full Document

UA PSY 325 - Language in Context

Download Language in Context
Our administrator received your request to download this document. We will send you the file to your email shortly.
Loading Unlocking...
Login

Join to view Language in Context and access 3M+ class-specific study document.

or
We will never post anything without your permission.
Don't have an account?
Sign Up

Join to view Language in Context 2 2 and access 3M+ class-specific study document.

or

By creating an account you agree to our Privacy Policy and Terms Of Use

Already a member?